# Load markdown file that contains the task and data description
filename         <- "Task_and_Data_Description.md"
data_description <- readChar(filename, file.info(filename)$size, )

Task

 

  • Participants performed an affective priming task that comprised a speeded go/no-go task with an embedded word categorization task. Responses in the go/no-go task served as a primes, the subsequently presented affective words as targets. Response facilitation in the word categorization task when preceded by an action in the go/no-go task of the same assigned valence will be referred to as affective priming effect. We assessed participants’ accuracy and response time (RT) in the go/no-go task and the word categorization task.

 

  • Go/no-go Task
    • A white arrow pointing upward or downward either turned
      • green and kept its initial orientation (2/3 of the trials; go trials)
      • turquoise and kept its orientation (1/6 of the trials; no-go trials), or
      • green but reversed its orientation (1/6 of the trials; no-go trials).
    • Participants were instructed to respond to go trials, but not to no-go trials.
    • Four response types in the go/no-go task were differentiated:
      • SH: slow hits (i.e., correct responses in go trials above the individual RT limit)
      • FH: fast hits (i.e., correct responses in go trials below the individual RT limit)
      • FA: false alarms (i.e., erroneous responses in no-go trials)
      • IR: inhibited responses (i.e., successful response inhibitions in no-go trials)

 

  • Word categorization task
    • After each go/no-go trial, an affective word was presented, which the participants were asked to categorize as either positive or negative.

 

  • During the task, the skin conductance response (SCR) was recorded.
    • For statistical analyses, the integrated SCR (ISCR; Benedek & Kaernbach, 2010) was extracted within a time window from 1 to 4 s after the participant’s response in the go/no-go task.

 

Hypotheses

 

  • We predicted that after false alarms in no-go trials, participants would respond faster and more accurately to negative words than to positive words. After fast hits in go trials, participants were expected to respond faster to positive words than to negative words (Aarts et al., 2012, 2013).
  • We expected that, relative to correct responses, incorrect responses would be associated with an increased SCR. We further predicted that SCR and affective priming effect (see above) might be positively related on a trial-by-trial level, as they are both manifestations of the emotional significance of an action (Aarts et al., 2013; Hajcak et al., 2003).
  • In addition, we assessed post-error slowing (PES) to examine whether PES would be associated with either affective priming or autonomic arousal. In line with accounts proposing that PES reflects an orienting response following error commission (Notebaert et al., 2009), a relation between SCR, which is a component of the orienting response, and PES could be expected.

 

Design

 

  • 4 x 2 design: go/no-go response type (SH, FH, FA, IR) and word valence (positive, negative) as within-participant factors

 

Data Description

 

  • Data set: ‘Single_Trial_Data.rda’
  • 15480 observations (30 participants, 516 trials per participant), 16 variables

 

VARIABLE DESCRIPTION VALUES
participant_id Participant identifier 1 to 30
trial Trial number within the task 1 to 516 per participant
block Experimental block 1 to 9 per participant
gng_response_type Response type in the go/no-go task SH = slow hit
FH = fast hit
FA = false alarm
IR = inhibited response
miss = missing response in go trial
wrong_key = response made with a word categorization key
gng_rt RT in the go/no-go task RT in milliseconds
NAs for trials in which no response was made (IR, miss)
gng_rt_invalid Indication whether RT in the go/no-go task was < 100 ms or > 700 ms TRUE
FALSE
NAs for trials in which no response was made (IR, miss)
gng_rt_inverse Inverse-transformed RT in the go/no-go task -1000/RT in milliseconds
NAs for trials in which no response was made (IR, miss)
word Affective word presented in the word categorization task 60 German words (30 positive, 30 negative) from the Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (Vo et al., 2009)
word_valence Valence of the affective word pos = positive
neg = negative
word_accuracy Correctness of the word categorization correct = correct categorization
incorrect = incorrect categorization
miss = missing response
wrong_key = response made with the go/no-go key
word_rt RT in the word categorization task RT in milliseconds
NAs for trials in which no response was made (miss)
word_rt_outlier Indication whether RT in the word categorization task was > 3 median absolute deviations (MAD) above or below a participant’s median RT per specific condition (gng_response_type x word_valence) TRUE
FALSE
NAs for trials in which no response was made (miss)
word_rt_inverse Inverse-transformed RT in the word categorization task -1000/RT in milliseconds
NAs for trials in which no response was made (miss)
iscr_gng_resp Integrated SCR extracted within a time window from 1 to 4 s after the response in the go/no-go task ISCR in microsiemens
trial_followed_or_preceded_
by_any_incorr_resp
Indication whether trial was followed / preceded by any incorrect response in the go/no-go or the word categorization task TRUE
FALSE

 

References

 

Aarts, K., De Houwer, J., & Pourtois, G. (2012). Evidence for the automatic evaluation of self-generated actions. Cognition, 124(2), 117-127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.009
Aarts, K., De Houwer, J., & Pourtois, G. (2013). Erroneous and correct actions have a different affective valence: Evidence from ERPs. Emotion, 13(5), 960-973. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032808s
Benedek, M., & Kaernbach, C. (2010). A continuous measure of phasic electrodermal activity. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 190(1), 80-91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.028
Hajcak, G., McDonald, N., & Simons, R. F. (2003). To err is autonomic: Error-related brain potentials, ANS activity, and post-error compensatory behavior. Psychophysiology, 40(6), 895-903. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-8986.00107
Notebaert, W., Houtman, F., Opstal, F. V., Gevers, W., Fias, W., & Verguts, T. (2009). Post-error slowing: An orienting account. Cognition, 111(2), 275-279. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.02.002
Vo, M. L., Conrad, M., Kuchinke, L., Urton, K., Hofmann, M. J., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). The Berlin Affective Word List Reloaded (BAWL-R). Behavior Research Methods, 41(2), 534-538. https://doi.org/10.3758/BRM.41.2.534



Session Info

sessionInfo()
## R version 3.6.1 (2019-07-05)
## Platform: x86_64-w64-mingw32/x64 (64-bit)
## Running under: Windows 10 x64 (build 18363)
## 
## Matrix products: default
## 
## locale:
## [1] LC_COLLATE=German_Germany.1252  LC_CTYPE=German_Germany.1252   
## [3] LC_MONETARY=German_Germany.1252 LC_NUMERIC=C                   
## [5] LC_TIME=German_Germany.1252    
## 
## attached base packages:
## [1] stats     graphics  grDevices datasets  utils     methods   base     
## 
## loaded via a namespace (and not attached):
##  [1] compiler_3.6.1  magrittr_1.5    htmltools_0.4.0 tools_3.6.1    
##  [5] yaml_2.2.0      Rcpp_1.0.5      stringi_1.4.3   rmarkdown_1.16 
##  [9] knitr_1.29      stringr_1.4.0   xfun_0.16       digest_0.6.22  
## [13] rlang_0.4.8     renv_0.12.0     evaluate_0.14